Unit of Analysis
If the interest is in analysis of data from dyads, you might want to click here to see a book that discusses that topic: Dyadic Data Analysis, by Kenny, Kashy, and Cook (2006).
This page provides the practicing researcher with guidance concerning the choice of the unit in the statistical analysis. I thank Charles Judd for helping me with many of the ideas on this page.
Outline
Statement
of the Problem
Independence
of Units
Unit
of Generalization
Unit
of Measurement
Unit
of Assignment or Sampling
How
Do I Conduct the Analysis?
References
Statement of
the Problem
Typically, the data structure is a matrix in which the row are called units, usually persons, and the columns are called variables. For such a data structure, if a statistical analyses were undertaken, row, usually person, would be the unit of analysis
Very often there is ambiguity as to what should be the unit of analysis for the statistical analysis. Many research studies have units broken into clusters:
In each case the unit of analysis might be persons or it might be clusters. To make cluster the unit of analysis, the researcher would compute the mean of persons within each cluster and use those means in the analysis.
With clustering persons are said to be nested within clusters. Alternatively, the two different units might be crossed; that is all combination of the two units might be created:
In this case, there are three different possibilities for the unit of analysis. Consider for instance judges rating faces. The three possible units are judge, face, and judge by face. In the remainder of this page, only the nesting of units is discussed, and the presumption is that persons are nested in groups.
Independence
of Units
Essential to a statistical analysis is the idea of replication or the repeated observation of a phenomenon.
For a replication to be a true replication, there must be independence
of observations. Independence of observations is presumed in standard measures of variability of observations.
For there to be independence, two observations are no more likely to be
similar (or different) than any other two observations. There are
several factors that make units nonindependent (Kenny & Judd, 1986).
Observations can be nonindependent because of compositional effects, common
fate, and social interaction:
To determine the unit of analysis, an assessment of whether observations are independent is often helpful. That is, the observations that are thought to be nonindependent, may in fact be independent. The measurement of nonindependence can be complicated, but in many cases an intraclass correlation (ICC) can be used to measure the degree of nonindependence. For the nested design, the ICC can be measured using a one-way analysis of variances, where the independent variable is group. (Read more about about the ICC measure for dyads.) Kenny and Judd (1996) discuss a wide variety of measures of nonindependence.
If it can be shown that units are independent by determining that the ICC is essentially zero, then person and not group can be the unit of analysis. If, however, the ICC is nonzero, then person should not be the unit of analysis.
Unit of Generalization
Another factor in deciding the unit of analysis is the
level of generalization that the researcher seeks to make. Consider a researcher who
measures 10 children in 10 classrooms from 10 different schools, or 1000 children in all.
There are three possible levels of generalizations: the student, the classroom,
and the school. One simple rule is to conduct the analysis at the level
at which one wants to make generalizations. So if one wants to draw
conclusions about persons, person should be the unit of analysis.
However, as will be seen, this simple rule cannot always be followed.
The researcher should be aware of the ecological fallacy (Robinson, 1950). The conclusions drawn from an analysis conducted at a group level may not apply at the individual level. Conversely, analyses at the individual level may not apply to the group level. In principal, the analysis should be conducted at the level at which generalizations should be made. However, there are exceptions to this rule.
Unit of Measurement
Another consideration is the unit of measurement. Again
returning to the example of children, classroom, and school, some variables
may be measured on children (e.g., achievement), some on the classroom
(e.g., teacher's gender), and some on the school (e.g., school size).
Just because one measures a variable at a certain level does not imply
that the variable operates at that level. Consider the variable group
size. Presumably this variable operates at the group level.
However, if a researcher changed the unit of measurement of the variable
and asked persons how big the group was, the variable would
operate at the group level, not at the individual level.
A related issue is that sometimes a researcher aggregates across units (i.e., averages) and so changes the unit of measurement. For example, to measure organizational climate, the mean of individual measures might be used. Just because the mean is at the level of the organization, does not mean that it, in fact, operates at that level.
Unit of Assignment
or Sampling
A final consideration in the decision about the unit
of analysis is design factors. It is necessary to consider the unit
by which observations are selected to enter the study or are assigned to
levels of the independent variable. A good idea is to perform the
statistical analysis at the level of the selection or assignment. So, for
instance, if floors in a dormitory are assigned to experimental conditions,
dormitory floor, not person, should be the unit of analysis. This is not
a "hard-and-fast rule," just a helpful guideline. For instance, individuals may be the unit of assignment, but if individuals interact with one another, then it may not be possible to use individual as the unit of analysis.
How Do I Conduct
the Analysis?
As discussed in Kenny (1995), there are three major approaches to the unit of analysis
question when persons are nested within groups (or observations are nested
within persons). First discussed are two approaches:
Sometimes, rules about the unit of assignment and the unit of generalization will be violated. For instance, classrooms may be the unit of assignment, but if there is no evidence of nonindependence due to classroom, person can be the unit of analysis. Alternatively, if there is evidence that classrooms are nonindependent, then person should not be the unit of analysis, even if person is the unit of generalization. Because all of the variation of treatment is between classrooms (recall that classroom is the unit of assignment), then the treatment's effect will be seen in between classroom variation, not within classroom.
The third strategy discussed in Kenny (1999) is the combined or pooled analysis: multilevel modeling essentially combines the two above strategies. In essence, it solves the unit of analysis question by making it a pseudo question. All the observations are analyzed, and the degree of nonindependence is empirically estimated. Judd and Kenny (2024) discuss how mixed models can be used in this combined analysis. Currently, this is strategy preferred by most analysts, and researchers should learn this approach.
Judd, C. M., & Kenny, D. A. (2024). Random factors and research generalization. In H. T. Reis, C. M. Judd, T. V. West (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology, 3rd ed.. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Kenny, D. A., & Judd, C. M. (1986). Consequences of violating the independence assumption in analysis of variance. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 422-431.
Kenny, D. A., & Judd, C. M. (1996). A general procedure for the estimation of interdependence. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 138-148.
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social psychology. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 233-265). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
Robinson, W. S. (1950). Ecological correlations and the behavior of individuals. American Sociological Review, 15, 351-357.