David A. Kenny
April 4, 2021
Chapter 2 of Interpersonal
Perception: The Foundation of Social Relationships.
First
impression research is of two types zero acquaintance
and thin slices. Zero acquaintance, a term coined by
Linda Albright, Thomas Malloy and myself in 1988, refers to the condition in
which the perceiver views the target, but the target is not interacting with
anyone. Many zero-acquaintance studies create groups of strangers on the first
day of class and have them make judgments of each other. Zero acquaintance has
the least information about the target.
A thin-slice study, a term coined by Nalani Ambady and Robert Rosenthal, involves the perceiver viewing the target for a brief amount of time, usually no more than a few minutes. Many thin slices studies use videos to present target information.
To what extent are first impressions consensual (two perceivers have similar
first impressions)? So are Jane and Helen's first impressions of Sally similar.
The measure of consensus is the proportion variance due to the target.
Alternatively, it can be viewed at the correlation between two different
perceivers of a set of targets.
The second question is to what extent are first impressions accurate? What is correlation of the target effect with the truth? An obvious but difficult question is what counts as the truth More on this topic when target accuracy is discussed, but here the truth is defined as the target's self-rating, ratings by peers, or the target's behavior.
The third question discussed here the stability of first impression. Is a first impression a lasting perception?
It might be thought at zero acquaintance that the perceiver does not know
anything about the target. However, the perceiver knows the target's age,
gender, race, dress, and physical attractiveness. Also known is how the target
moves and the voice contain information. This information can be used to make
judgments of personality through stereotypes. For instance, at least in western
cultures, physical attractiveness is associated with judgments of extroversion
whereas in China physical attractiveness is associated with judgments of
intelligence. For there to be consensus at zero acquaintance, there must
be shared beliefs about links between physical appearance and nonverbal
behavior with personality. These shared beliefs about targets are called stereotypes.
Appearance information and nonverbal behaviors are associated with different
personality traits. Lay people believe that only bigoted people use
stereotypes, but social psychological research has documented that everyone
uses stereotypes and much of that use is unconscious.
The lay view is that stereotypes are inherently false which is the case that not all members of the social group have the trait. However, some stereotypes are true of some people creating what is called a kernel of truth.
Glen Cleeton and F. B. Knight in 1924 were interested in phrenology,
the “science” of predicting personality by measurements of the
head. As participants, they had 20 women who were members of two
sororities and 10 men who were members of a fraternity. In groups of 10
same-gendered persons, they were placed on stage and were rated on eight traits
by 70 people who were described as “casual observers” and were composed of
“businessmen, school superintendents and students of personnel management” (p.
223-224). The eight traits were Sound Judgment, Intellectual Capacity,
Frankness, Will Power, Ability to Make Friends, Leadership, Originality, and
Impulsiveness. The average level of consensus between two judges was .20,
meaning that about twenty percent of variance is due to target. To assess the
validity of these judgements, Cleeton and Knight used peer ratings as the
measure of the truth and found an average correlation of xx.
The second major study of zero acquaintance that I know of was published by Warren Norman and Lou Goldberg in 1966. They had University of Michigan students rate each other's personality on the first day of class. The study yielded two sets of very surprising results. First, students agreed with one another about each other's personality. Second, and even more surprisingly, these ratings by virtual strangers agreed with the targets' self-ratings. So if two people agreed that a third person was friendly, that person tended to see him or herself as friendly. Thus, these judgments at zero acquaintance are consensual and appear to be valid.
Perhaps because of its counter-intuitive nature, these findings demonstrated by Norman and Goldberg study were virtually totally ignored. In 1988 at the University of Connecticut, Linda Albright, Thomas Malloy, and myself published three studies of zero acquaintance using the paradigm that Norman and Goldberg developed. (You can download the three raw data sets from this study: MALZER, KENZER, and ZERO.) We had students on the first day of class form a circle and rate each other. We replicated the results of Norman and Goldberg. Across three separate studies with 259 participants, there was consensus on the dimensions of Extroversion and Conscientiousness and correlations between perceiver judgments with and self-ratings; we, however, used a more elaborate statistical model (the Social Relations Model).
Since the publication of the Albright, Malloy, and Kenny study, there have been numerous follow-up studies, done at the University of Connecticut and around the world. Each of these studies shows evidence of agreement in ratings and correlations with self-ratings. In Kenny, Horner, Kashy, and Chu (1992), we studied zero acquaintance in more controlled settings. Participants viewed 20 second video clips in a laboratory setting. We were able to replicate the basic results of consensus being highest for judgments of Extroversion and somewhat weaker for Conscientiousness.
Several important studies were conducted by Peter Borkenau and Annette
Liebler (1992) in Germany. They undertook special measures to ensure that
the targets and judges are unacquainted. Additionally, they used a community
sample of targets, not just college students as have been used in other
studies. Borkenau and Liebler find even stronger levels of agreement or
consensus, likely because a community sample would be more heterogeneous than a
sample of college students. They also consistently found evidence of
correlations between zero-acquaintance ratings and self-ratings. They have also
done the most detailed investigation of cues used to make personality judgments
at zero acquaintance. Here is a brief summary of what they found:
Extroversion: friendly
expression, attractive, made-up face
Agreeableness:
feminine, short strides, effortful reading
Conscientiousness:
effortful reading, not relaxed sitting, formal dress
Emotional Stability:
masculine, light garments, stiff walking
Culture: informal
dress, weak voice, soft voice
Maurice Levesque and myself (1993) examined more closely the finding that perceivers generally agree about who is and is not extroverted and included a behavioral measure of accuracy. Participants formed groups of unacquainted women and without interacting each woman rated the other group members on traits indicating the Big 5 factors and made a series of behavioral predictions (e.g., talkativeness). We found that not only was there consensus on rating of talkativeness but when the women were videotaped interacting one-on-one with each other women who were judged as more talkative actually talked more.
Only one published zero-acquaintance study involved non-western subjects (Albright, Malloy, Dong, Kenny, Fang, Winquist, & Yu, 1997). This study was conducted at Beijing Normal University in China. Although some results were somewhat different, they were basically consistent with results from studies of persons from western cultures: Perceivers agree in their ratings at zero acquaintance and these rating correlate with self-ratings.
Research has consistently shown that agreement at zero acquaintance is strongest for Extroversion. Remember that people know how talkative someone is, even if they have never seen the person engage in conversation. Agreement is generally shown for ratings of Conscientiousness. Agreement for Agreeableness, Emotional Stability, and Culture tend to be less. As for validity, it seems to be greatest for Conscientiousness. Recall, however, that Borkenau and Liebler find even stronger levels of agreement and validity with a community sample.
Fortunately, there is a zero‑acquaintance study of liking conducted by Mitja Back, Stefan Schmukle, and Boris Egloff conducted in 2011. They took all 74 of the students intending to study psychology at the Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz in Germany. They did find some agreement of .14 for liking.
Although correlations of ratings at zero acquaintance with self-ratings suggest that the zero-acquaintance ratings are valid, there are alternative explanations. For instance, it might be that a person successfully conveys a false image through clothing and speech. For instance, someone who falsely thinks he or she is athletic wears athletic clothing and perceivers mistakenly conclude that the person is athletic. Stronger evidence that zero-acquaintance ratings are accurate is needed.
The Levesque and Kenny 1993 study done at the University of Connecticut provides to date the strongest foundation of validity. Zero acquaintance ratings of Extroversion predicted how much each woman talked and how much each gestured. Many of these correlations were very strong.
The bulk of the research on accuracy based on minimal information is
within the thin slices tradition. In the original 1992 thin slice
meta-analysis, Ambady and Rosenthal found an average correlation of .40.
Sam Gosling, Sei Jin Ko, Thomas Mannarelli, and Margaret Morris (2002) had individuals make judgments about a target after seeing the targets' office or bedroom. They showed that strangers could reliable and validly judge targets personalities by looking at their bedrooms and offices. Consensus and accuracy correlations were generally stronger than those found in zero-acquaintance research. This study demonstrates that accuracy is possible without ever meeting (or even seeing) a person.
Nalini Ambady, Mark Hallahan, and Brett Connor (1998) have demonstrated consensus and better than chance accuracy at judging sexual orientation on the basis of 10 second and 1 second video clips, still photographs, and even a 10 second figural outline display. Evidence suggests that gay men and lesbians are more accurate than heterosexuals at judging sexual orientation in some conditions (still photographs and 1 second clips) but not others (10 second clips).
In 2015, Alexander Todorov and colleagues have questioned the validity
of thin slices and zero-acquaintance research. They give a 1928 quote
from Clark Hull which pretty much summarizes their views: “The results as a
whole certainly look very bad for the judgment of character on the basis of
photographs” (p. 119). To my mind their objections do not invalidate the
accuracy findings reviewed above, but readers should draw their own
conclusions. Please note that the focus of Todorov and colleagues is on
the ability of perceivers to make predictions about a target from the face, but
they criticize studies like Borkenau and Liebler which did not just use
faces. They make five points, each of which is considered.
The first is that many researchers have failed to properly control for
gender, ethnicity, and age. The suggestion is that when such controls are
made, the accuracy would disappear. This is an issue in some of the studies,
but for most of the studies it is not an issue. For example, in the 2002 Sam Gosling
and colleagues' study when they controlled for gender and ethnicity, the basic
pattern of results remained, essentially unchanged.
The second issue is that they view above-chance accuracy as a rather
feeble benchmark which says little about the validity of these sorts of
judgments. The reader can decide this for him or herself. To my
mind, showing any validity of first impressions is impressive. It is not
clear to me why the bar needs to be raised, but the reader may feel
differently.
The third issue is that perceivers do not seem to be aware of any skill
of this area. To back up this claim they look at relationship between who
is better at making these sorts of judgments and their awareness of the
skill. As discussed in the Individual Differences in
Accuracy page, it is very likely that if there are differences that they
are very small. But even if there are differences, why is it important
that perceivers be self-aware of their skill?
The fourth issue is the inherent variation of facial
images. If a picture is taken of a target, one picture may look
very different from another picture. Consider the famous mug shots of
Nick Nolte and Mel Gibson after their arrests. Looking at them you do not
see anything near a matinee idol. However, the argument makes any
correlation of judgments with the truth even more impressive, because of the
inherent variability of faces. For this to be an issue, the researcher
would have “to cheat” by selecting photographs to confirm their
hypothesis. This argument would seem to have little merit. From
Cleeton and Knight to the zero acquaintance studies of the 1980s, live targets
were used and so there was no selection of facial images by researchers to
confirm their hypothesis.
The fifth issue is that even if there is accuracy, that accuracy does
not imply a direct biological link between facial features and
personality. That link might be mediated by the self‑fulfilling
prophecy which was discussed above. I have some trouble understanding
this objection. Just because Todorov and colleagues want a biological
link, why should I have to care? In sum, I do not find the
arguments made by Todorov and colleagues to be persuasive.
Certainly there are limits of the conclusion of the accuracy of first impressions. First realize that the accuracy correlations reported above refer to the average judgment (the crowd), not the accuracy of an individual judgment. Second, there have been studies that have looked for accuracy of impression based on minimal information, shoes.
There is the old saying that a first impression is a lasting
impression. In part this is due to the fact that if a target makes a poor
first impression, the perceiver will likely decide to avoid the target, and
that target never has a chance to make a better second impression.
However, what does happen if the target gets a second chance? Is it the
case that the second impression is not all that different from the first
impression?
The 1992 paper that I wrote with Caryl Horner, Ling-chuan Chu, and
Deborah Kashy in examined the stability of first impressions from zero
acquaintance to interacting with the target for about ten minutes across two
studies. We examined Extroversion and found strong stability for the
target effect, r = .90, but not nearly the
same degree of stability for the relationship effect, r
= .40. This finding then is that if a person makes a poor first
impression and perceivers agree, that component does not change much over
time. However, if the perceiver sees the target in an idiosyncratic way,
that part does change quite a bit.
However, the more interesting question concerns evaluation. If initially somebody evaluates a target negatively, can the target improve his or her impression after more interaction? Two very different studies examined this hypothesis. The first study is near and dear to the hearts of many of the readers as it concerns teacher evaluations. If as a teacher, you make a bad first, can you overcome it if you are given a second chance? In 2015, Jennifer Gross and colleagues asked 145 college students to evaluate 10 teachers based on a thin-slice of 6-minute video of 10 professors lecturing. Then three to ten weeks later, students listened live to each professor lecturing for 40 minutes and evaluated him or her. Both the target and the relationship components in these evaluations can be computed at each time and their stability over time can be assessed. The target effect, how positively the teacher is evaluated, was highly stable, r = .86. Thus, if a teacher made a poor impression in the short lecture to students, about 93 percent of the time he or she again made a poor impression when given a second chance. However, the relationship effect was not nearly as stable , r = .24.
What about interpersonal liking? If initially you dislike
someone, what are the chances that it will change? Returning to the study
conducted by Mitja Back and colleagues in 2000 where liking was measured for 73
college students at zero acquaintance in the beginning of the academic
year. Liking was then re-measured for 54 students at the end of the
academic year, about nine months later. The stability correlation of the
target effect is .42 and the correlation for the relationship is .23.
The conclusion is straightforward: The consensual part of the
first impression is very slowly changing, but the non-consensual part does
change.
Albright, L., Kenny, D. A., & Malloy, T. E. (1988). Consensus in
personality judgments at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 55, 387-395.
Albright, L., Malloy, T. E., Dong, Q., Kenny, D. A., Fang, X., Winquist,
L., & Yu, D. (1997). Cross-cultural consensus in personality judgments. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 558-569.
Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Conner, B. (1999). Accuracy of judgments
of sexual orientation from thin slices of behavior. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 77, 538-547.
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive
behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 111, 256-274.
Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2010). From
first sight to friendship:
A longitudinal social relations analysis of stability and change in
interpersonal attraction. Unpublished manuscript, Department of
Psychology, Münster
University, Münster, Germany. Retrieved from https://osf.io/97ru8.
Borkenau, P. & Liebler, A. (1992). Trait inferences: Sources of
validity at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 62, 645-657.
Cleeton, G. U., & Knight, F. B. (1924). Validity of character
judgments based on external criteria. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 8, 215-231.
Gosling, S., Ko, S. J., Mannarelli, R., & Morris, M. E. (2002).
A room with a cue: Personality judgments based on offices and bedrooms.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 379-398.
Kenny, D. A., Horner, C., Kashy, D. A., & Chu, L. (1992). Consensus
at zero acquaintance: Replication, behavioral cues, and stability. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 88-97.
Levesque, M. J., & Kenny, D. A. (1993). Accuracy of behavioral
predictions at zero acquaintance: A social relations analysis. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1178-1187.
Norman, W. T., & Goldberg, L. R. (1966). Raters, ratees, and
randomness in personality structure. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 4, 681-691.
Todorov, A., Olivola, C. Y., Dotsch, R., &
Mende-Siedlecki, P. (2015). Social attributions from faces: Determinants,
consequences, accuracy, and functional significance. Annual
Review of Psychology, 66,
519-545.